The Impact of Gender on the Acquisition of VOT of English Plosives Produced by Bilingual Emirati Children
Abstract
Gender differences play a crucial role in shaping bilingual phonological development, particularly in multilingual societies like the United Arab Emirates (UAE). This study explored the influence of gender on the acquisition of Voice Onset Time (VOT) for word-initial English plosives among bilingual Emirati preschool children, examining how boys and girls acquired and differentiated voicing contrasts in English as their second language (L2). The study included 20 typically developing Emirati children aged 2:00 years to 5:11 years, all native speakers of Emirati Arabic and bilinguals with English as their second language (L2). Data were collected through a picture-naming task in a controlled acoustic environment, and the audio recordings of voiced and voiceless plosives were analyzed using PRAAT software. The results revealed significant gender-based differences in VOT patterns. Boys displayed stronger interference from their first language (L1), Emirati Arabic, in their English plosive production, whereas girls exhibited VOT values more closely aligned with English norms. These findings suggest that gender influences bilingual phonetic development, potentially due to biological, social, and developmental factors. The study emphasizes the importance of incorporating gender as a variable in educational strategies and clinical interventions, providing valuable insights into bilingual phonological acquisition and its implications for language teaching and the assessment of phonological disorders.
Keywords: Voice onset time (VOT); bilingualism; gender differences; Emirati Arabic; phonetic acquisition
Full Text:
PDFReferences
Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice onset time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2017.05.003
Al Abed, I., & Vine, P. (2005). United Arab Emirates: A new perspective. Trident Press Ltd.
Al-Issa, A. (2019). English language teaching in the United Arab Emirates: Challenges and future directions. Learning and Teaching in Higher Education: Gulf Perspectives, 16(2), 33–48. https://doi.org/10.18538/lthe.v16.n2.272
Al-Issa, A., & Dahan, L. (2011). Global English and Arab learners: A sociolinguistic study of ESL teachers’ perceptions. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 1(3), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.4304/tpls.1.3.243-251
Alsuhaim, W. (2022). Gender differences in the acquisition of VOT of English bilabial stops among Saudi ESL learners. 3L: Language, Linguistics, Literature®️ The Southeast Asian Journal of English Language Studies, 28(3), 99–115. https://doi.org/10.17576/3L-2022-2803-08
AlShamsi, A. S., & Alsheikh, N. O. (2020). Sociocultural integration of bilingualism and biliteracy of Emiratis: Exploratory study in a group of fifth grade students. 3L: Language, Linguistics, Literature, 26(3), 66–80. https://doi.org/10.17576/3L-2020-2603-06
Al-Tai, F., & Kasim, Z. (2021). Acoustic analysis of VOT in Iraqi Arabic stops. Journal of Language and Linguistic Studies, 17(2), 689–703. https://doi.org/10.17263/jlls.889472
Al-Tamimi, F., & Khattab, G. (2011). VOT production in bilingual and monolingual Jordanians: Language contact and phonetic convergence. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(4), 446–470. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910371025
Al-Wer, E. (2024). Phonological conditioning of affricate variability in Emirati Arabic. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 54(1), 146–164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100323000166
Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2021). PRAAT: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.1.41) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org
Boyle, J. (2012). The challenge of teaching English in multilingual contexts: A case study from the UAE. TESOL Arabia Perspectives, 19(2), 18–23.
Buckingham, L. (2016). English as a lingua franca in the Gulf. World Englishes, 35(1), 104–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/weng.12140
Burman, D. D., Bitan, T., & Booth, J. R. (2008). Sex differences in neural processing of language among children. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1349–1362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.021
Byrd, D. (1992). Sex differences in vocal tract variations in vowel production. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(1), 300–311. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.402758
Eckert, P., & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2003). Language and gender. Cambridge University Press.
Fabiano Smith, L. (2019). Standardized tests and the diagnosis of speech sound disorders. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 4(1), 58–66. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_PERS-SIG1-2018-001
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Bunta, F. (2012). Voice onset time of voiceless bilabial and velar stops in 3-year-old bilingual children and their monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(2), 148–163. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2011.632943
Fabiano-Smith, L., & Goldstein, B. (2010). Phonological acquisition in bilingual Spanish–English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 160–178. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0135)
Federal Competitiveness and Statistics Centre. (2020). Annual report 2020: Population and demographics. Abu Dhabi: FCSC.
Flege, J. E. (1991). Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice-onset time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89(1), 395–411. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.400473
Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. A. (2011). What accounts for ‘age’ effects on second-language learning? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(3), 635–668. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263111000303
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. A. (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(5), 3125–3134. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.413041
Flege, J. E., & Port, R. (1981). Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 24(2), 125–146. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098102400203
Foulkes, P., Docherty, G. J., & Watt, D. (2005). Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language, 81(1), 177–206. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0010
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitudes and motivation. Edward Arnold.
Goldrick, M., Runnqvist, E., & Costa, A. (2014). Language switching makes pronunciation less nativelike. Psychological Science, 25(4), 1031–1036. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613518352
Goldstein, L. (2020). Phonological universals and language-specific variation. Annual Review of Linguistics, 6, 319–338. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011619-030525
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5
Hambly, C., Wren, Y., McLeod, S., & Roulstone, S. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on speech production: A systematic review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00178.x
Khattab, G. (2002). VOT production in English and Arabic bilingual and monolingual children. In A. Skarabela, S. Fish, & A. Do (Eds.), BUCLD 26: Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 336–347). Cascadilla Press.
Khattab, G. (2013). Phonetic convergence and divergence strategies in English-Arabic bilingual children. Linguistics, 51(2), 439–472. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2013-0017
Koenig, L. L. (2001). Laryngeal factors in voiceless consonant production in men, women, and 5-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(5), 1051–1066. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/083)
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830
Macnamara, J. (1967). The bilingual’s linguistic performance: A psychological overview. Journal of Social Issues, 23(2), 58–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1967.tb00522.x
McCarthy, M., Evans, G., & Giegerich, H. J. (2014). The Routledge handbook of phonological theory. Routledge.
Ochs, E. (1992). Indexing gender. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 335–358). Cambridge University Press.
Olson, D. J., & Hayes-Harb, R. (2019). VOT production and perception in Arabic-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(1), 88–102. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000561
Paradis, J. (2021). Bilingual children’s acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and L1 properties. Language Learning, 71(1), 169–211. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12426
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014662
Roberts, J. (1997). Acquisition of variable rules: A study of (-t,d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 24(2), 351–372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500099700306X
Saadah, E. (2011). Voice onset time production in Najdi Arabic and English: A cross-linguistic study of adult and child bilingual speakers. In E. Benmamoun (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XXII–XXIII (pp. 245–268). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/sal.1.12saa
Sony Corporation. (n.d.). ICD-PX370 digital voice recorder user guide. https://www.sony.com/electronics/support/articles/00008352
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Baum, S. R. (2006). Production of coronal stops by bilingual French-English children. Journal of Phonetics, 34(4), 499–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2005.10.001
Sweeting, P. M., & Baken, R. J. (1982). Voice onset time in normal subjects: A comparative study of two measurement techniques. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25(1), 99–109. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.2501.99
Tamim, A., & Hamann, S. (2021). Voice onset time in Palestinian Arabic: Effects of dialect, gender, and context. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 51(2), 259–276. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025100319000200
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 615–630. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.615
Troudi, S., & Jendli, A. (2011). Emirati students’ experiences of English as a medium of instruction. Learning and Teaching in Higher Education: Gulf Perspectives, 8(1).
Whiteside, S. P., & Marshall, J. (2001). Developmental trends in voice onset time: Some evidence for sex differences. Phonetica, 58(3), 196–210. https://doi.org/10.1159/000028712
Whiteside, S. P., & Marshall, J. (2001). Developmental trends in voice onset time: Some evidence for sex differences. Phonetica, 58(3), 196–210. https://doi.org/10.1159/000056199
Wilsenach, C., & Makaure, P. (2018). Gender effects on phonological processing and reading development in Northern Sotho children learning to read in English: A case study of Grade 3 learners. South African Journal of Childhood Education, 8(1), Article a546. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajce.v8i1.546
Yang, C.-L., & Fox, R. A. (2014). The effect of dialect and bilingualism on VOT production in Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of Phonetics, 47, 279–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.10.005
Younes, R., & Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (2022). Cross-linguistic influence on voice onset time in Spanish-English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(2), 357–372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000186
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.17576/3L-2025-3102-12
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.
eISSN : 2550-2247
ISSN : 0128-5157